As indicated, consumer researchers who have been influenced by particular fields
have tended to work on different topics and employ different research methods. In
this section we explore two contrasts: (a) BDT versus social cognition consumer
research and (b) positivist versus postmodern consumer research.
The Behavioral Decision Theory and Social-Cognition Approaches to Consumer
Research Whereas the distinction between social and cognitive research does
not play nearly as significant a role in consumer research as it does in psychology,
the somewhat loose distinction between social cognition–based research and so-called BDT is more prominent in consumer research. In psychology, BDT accounts
for a relatively small segment of the literature, although leading BDT researchers
such as Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky have had great impact on both the
cognitive and social psychological literatures.
In the consumer behavior literature,
research that follows the BDT research paradigm has accounted for a large and
growing proportion of all nonpostmodern articles published in the leading journals.
Indeed, the central BDT issues of judgment and choice are directly relevant to the
most researched area in marketing and consumer behavior, namely, influences on
purchase decisions. Furthermore, BDT research serves as a bridge between the
“behavioral” and “quantitative” sides of marketing because both BDT and quan-titative research share the link to economics and the focus on consumer choice.
Conversely, quantitative research in marketing tends to have less in common with
social cognition consumer research (and even less with postmodern research).
First, it should be emphasized that social cognition and BDT researchers in
marketing share many of the same research values and methods. For example, in the debate between the positivist and postmodern approaches to research, both
social cognition and BDT researchers, by and large, are on the same (positivist)
side. Furthermore, there are differences in emphasis among researchers within the
consumer BDT and social cognition subfields, making it difficult to generalize
regarding differences between the two camps. In particular, much BDT con-sumer research has investigated the processes underlying judgments and decisions
(e.g. Coupey 1994, Dhar & Nowlis 1999, Sen & Johnson 1997).
Conversely, other
BDT research has focused more on judgment and decision-making phenomena,
such as the manner in which consumers integrate the opinions of multiple critics
(e.g. West & Broniarczyk 1998) or the impact of anticipating regret (e.g. Simon-son 1992), where process measures are either not used at all or provide additional
insights but are not the focus of the research. Similarly, there are large differ-ences among social cognition consumer researchers with respect to both research
methods and topics (see, e.g. Kisielius & Sternthal 1984 and Tybout et al 1983,
compared with Alba et al 1999 and Lynch et al 1988).
With the caveat that there are exceptions to each of the following generalizations,
there are several key differences between BDT and social cognition consumer
research:
1. One obvious difference relates to the primary influences on each area.
Social cognition consumer research has been influenced primarily by social
cognition research in psychology (e.g. Chaiken 1980, Fiske & Taylor
1984). Conversely, the primary influence on BDT consumer research has
been the BDT literature, including the work of Kahneman & Tversky
(e.g. 1979), Thaler (1985), and other researchers. Furthermore, similar
to BDT research published in nonmarketing journals, BDT consumer
research has tended to use the normative benchmark of value maximization
and time-consistent preferences for evaluating the significance of research
findings. Thus, findings that demonstrate violations of the classical
economic assumptions regarding buyer behavior have typically been
regarded as interesting and important. For example, BDT consumer
researchers have demonstrated that, (a) the framing of product attributes
(e.g. ground beef that is “80% lean” or has “20% fat”) influences product
evaluation even after actual experience (“80% lean beef” tasted better than
“20% fat beef;” Gaeth & Levin 1988); (b) when costs significantly precede
benefits, the sunk cost effect is greatly diminished (Gourville & Soman
1998); (c) the interaction between the pleasure of consumption and the
pain of paying has predictable impact on consumer behavior and hedonics
(Prelec & Loewenstein 1998); and (d) preference elicitation tasks involving
comparison of options (e.g. choice), judgments of individual options
(e.g. ratings), and matching of two options varying in price and quality,
produce systematically different preferences (e.g. Carmon & Simonson
1998, Hsee & Leclerc 1998, Nowlis & Simonson 1997). In recent years,
the focus has shifted from demonstrations of value maximization violations to studies that are designed to gain a better understanding of the factors that
influence the construction of preferences (e.g. Drolet et al 2000; for a
review, see Bettman et al 1998).
2. Social cognition and BDT consumer research have tended to build on
different underlying models of buyer behavior and the communication
process. One often referenced response hierarchy model (e.g. McGuire
1969), which has its origin in the communications area, includes the
following stages: exposure/attention!reception/encoding!cognitive
response!attitude!intention!behavior. The other model, which
focuses on consumer decision making (or buying process), includes the
following stages (e.g. Peter & Olson 1993): problem recognition!
information search!evaluation of alternatives!purchase
decision/choice!postpurchase evaluation. Although these two models
highlight somewhat different elements in the consumer response and
decision-making process (e.g. attention and intention versus search and
evaluation) and employ different terminology, the essential components are
quite similar. Thus, for example, most studies that examine influences on
attitudes and attitude change also effectively investigate the formation of
preferences and alternative evaluation, and vice versa (e.g. Morwitz et al
1993). However, whereas social cognition consumer research has focused
on the stages in the communications (or hierarchy-of-effects) model and on
how judgments and attitudes are formed, BDT consumer research has
tended to examine the decision-making model and particularly the
determinants of choice.
3. Related to item 2, whereas BDT consumer researchers have studied
primarily stimulus-based phenomena (e.g. Dhar 1997, Kahn & Louie
1990), social cognition research has focused more on memory-based tasks
(e.g. Alba et al 1991; Biehal & Chakravarti 1982, 1983). For example,
building on the influential accessibility-diagnosticity model of Feldman &
Lynch (1988), Lynch et al (1988) proposed that decisions arise from a
process whereby inputs are sequentially retrieved from memory, with the
consumer updating the implications of already considered evidence with
each new input retrieved. The order of retrieval is a function of the
accessibility of each input, but accessible information can be actively
disregarded if it is perceived to be nondiagnostic.
4. Although there are certainly exceptions to this generalization, BDT
research has tended to focus more on substantive phenomena, which are
explained based on existing theories or lead to theoretical extensions.
Conversely, a greater share of social cognition consumer research has
involved theory testing and extensions that have implications for the
consumer environment (e.g. Ratneshwar & Chaiken 1991).
5. There are also differences in the process measures that social cognition and
BDT consumer researchers tend to use. Social cognition researchers often employ measures such as cognitive response (e.g. Sternthal et al 1978) and
recall to gain insights into cognitive processes. In the BDT area,
researchers who have used process measures tended to rely primarily on
measures of information acquisition, verbal protocols, and response time
(e.g. Bettman & Park 1980, Brucks 1988, Johnson 1984, Sen & Johnson
1997).
There is no obvious explanation for the different process measures
employed. Finally, the use of mediation and path analysis is more common
in social cognition than in BDT consumer research.
Despite the differences between the social cognition and BDT approaches to
consumer research, the main topics of investigation are closely related and the re-
search methods, by and large, are similar. Thus, decreasing the division between
the two areas and increasing communication and collaboration can advance the
consumer research field. Consider, for example, the currently dominant view of
consumer persuasion based on the elaboration likelihood model (e.g. Petty et al
1983). Briefly, evaluation of arguments presented in an ad is a frequent example
of processing through the central route, whereas the impact of background mu-
sic represents an example of persuasion through the peripheral route. However,
depending on the motivation and ability to process the information, the same cue
(e.g. the product endorser) might influence persuasion through either the central
or peripheral route (e.g. Petty et al 1991).
In the BDT literature researchers have made a related distinction between com-
pensatory and heuristic-based decision rules (see, e.g. Bettman 1979). This dis-
tinction, however, refers typically to the manner in which consumers process
attribute information, as opposed to different types of information. Interestingly,
BDT researchers have not paid much attention to the manner in which typical
peripheral cues, such as source characteristics and background music, impact con-
sumer preferences. On the other hand, BDT consumer researchers have studied
extensively the impact of various contextual factors, such as task characteristics
(e.g. Fischer et al 1999) and the configuration of the option set under consideration
(e.g. Huber et al 1982, Huber & Puto 1983, Wernerfelt 1995), on consumer pref-
erences.
These contextual moderators have not received much attention in social
cognition research on attitude and persuasion, even though such factors appear
quite relevant to our understanding of attitude and persuasion.
Another area that could potentially benefit from increased integration of BDT
and social cognition research involves the BDT notion of construction of pref-
erences and the related concepts in social cognition of attitude accessibility and
diagnosticity (e.g. Fazio et al 1989, Feldman & Lynch 1988), as well as the notions
of attitude strength and ambivalence (e.g. Priester & Petty 1996). For example,
Krosnick & Shuman (1988) showed that, contrary to common assumptions, mea-
sured attitudes of individuals whose attitudes are intense, important, and held with
certainty, are just as susceptible to response order effects as other respondents’.
A related finding from BDT consumer research is that expertise and involvement
(e.g. owing to accountability) often do not diminish and, in some cases, enhance the susceptibility of consumers to judgment and decision errors such as overconfi-dence and the attraction effect (e.g. Mahajan 1992, Simonson 1989; but see Coupey
et al 1998). Thus, by integrating findings from the social cognition and BDT
areas, we are likely to gain a better understanding of the moderators of consumer
susceptibility to various biases.
Another example of a finding in social cognition research that might have
significant BDT implications is the observation that stronger, more accessible
attitudes diminish sensitivity to changes in the attitude object (Fazio et al 2000).
In particular, it suggests that, although well-formed, stable preferences might
represent the ideal sovereign consumer, it might actually reduce consumer welfare
and choice effectiveness. Finally, social cognition research on the measurement
and construction of attitudes (e.g. Menon et al 1995; for a review, see Schwarz &
Bohner 2000) can have significant implications for decision research, and vice
versa. In sum, despite the differences in research traditions, issues, and methods,
we believe there is a need and opportunity for greater interaction and collaboration
between social cognition and BDT consumer researchers.